Lab Manager | Run Your Lab Like a Business

Exacerbating the Replication Crisis in Science: Replication Studies Are Often Unwelcome

Just three percent of psychology journals explicitly state that they accept replication studies, contributing to the replication crisis

by Frontiers
Register for free to listen to this article
Listen with Speechify
0:00
5:00

scientific journals

 

Researchers in London have investigated 1,151 psychology journals and found that just three percent state that they welcome scientists to submit replication studies for publication. In replication studies, scientists try to replicate the findings of previous studies to verify that their results are robust and correct.

Get training in Risk Communication and Decision-making and earn CEUs.One of over 25 IACET-accredited courses in the Academy.
Risk Communication and Decision-making Course

Publishing research findings in scientific journals is a career yardstick for many scientists, where the number and perceived impact of their publications are considered important measures of career success. This view is also frequently taken by the funding bodies and tenure committees that provide academic researchers with hotly contested research funding and job security.

Scientific journals provide a platform for scientists to communicate their research. The impact of published papers is often gauged by how many times they are cited (or referenced) by other papers, the idea being that such citations indicate that other scientists are building on the original work.

Scientific journals are also judged on the citations their published manuscripts receive, leading to metrics such as the impact factor. Put simply, the impact factor represents the average number of citations an article in a journal receives in a year. Journals with higher impact factors are often considered to be more desirable places to publish, although the impact factor has sometimes been criticized as an inaccurate measurement of quality.

The tendency for many journals to accept only papers that report positive and original findings has been termed publication bias. In traditional academic publishing, a small minority of submitted studies are accepted for publication, based on their perceived significance or originality, or if they confirm an existing hypothesis. Studies that are thought to provide only a small advance, or those that present "non-impactful" or negative results are frequently rejected.

In fact, many journals explicitly state in their aims or guide to authors that high levels of significance or originality are a prerequisite for publication. Journal editors often enforce extremely high rejection rates, the idea being that selective publishing will increase the journal's impact factor.

However, this traditional publishing model has drawn criticism that it may exclude studies with perceived low impact, but which are valuable for scientific integrity and development. Replications are one such type of study, where researchers attempt to replicate the findings of previous studies to verify that their results are robust and correct.

It is important that scientific experiments are repeatable and produce identical or similar results if repeated. Otherwise, it is difficult to know if experimental results are correct and reveal a real phenomenon, or if they are merely a one-off caused by experimental error or highly specific conditions that are difficult to recreate.

Related Article: Study That Undercut Psych Research Got It Wrong

"Science progresses through replication and contradiction. The former builds the body of evidence, the latter determines whether such a body exists," explains Professor Neil Martin, of Regent's University London, lead author on the study, recently published in Frontiers in Psychology.

Recently, a so-called "replication crisis" has been brewing in science. This is occurring across various fields, but the issue has recently come to a head following some high-profile failed replications in psychology. "Researchers have been accused of various creative methodological misdemeanors which may have led to false-positive results being published," says Martin. "We're still uncovering questionable research practices in some well-known historical studies, and I would not be surprised to see many others emerging."

There is a growing awareness and discussion in the wider scientific community that replications are not performed or published enough. This could potentially result in whole areas of scientific research being constructed on foundations of sand.

There are many reasons for the lack of replication studies in various fields of science. The overriding scientific culture is one of innovation, originality and discovery, and scientists may be reluctant to conduct "housekeeping" replication studies, when resources are limited.

Another potential reason is the perception that replications have limited impact, and will therefore be difficult to publish. "Journals have been criticized for not readily accepting replications, but the basis for this criticism is anecdotal," explains Martin.

To begin to quantify this phenomenon, Martin and Richard Clarke, a research student at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, investigated how welcoming psychology journals are to publishing replications. "We wanted to investigate whether journals specifically rejected (or did not recommend) the submission of replications. We did this by examining the aims and instructions to authors of 1151 journals in psychology," says Martin.

The team found that only three percent of the psychology journals on their list explicitly stated that they accepted replications. There was no difference between high and low impact journals and no difference between the different branches of psychology.

Thirty-three percent of the journals emphasized the need for scientific originality in submissions, which discourages scientists to submit replications, while 63 percent neither encouraged nor discouraged replications. The remaining one percent actively discouraged replications.

So how do we make psychology journals more welcoming to replications? "We've suggested that all journals in psychology should state that they accept replications that are positive and negative," explains Martin. "Researchers could also submit two papers for publication when they submit original research: one which reports the original results and one replication which acts as a test of the original findings."

An alternative approach to traditional publishing, in the form of impact-neutral publishers, could also help to increase the number of published replications. Impact-neutral publishers, such as Frontiers, PLOS ONE, and many BMC journals, don't make value judgements on perceived impact, and instead assess the scientific validity of a submitted study, when deciding whether to publish it. "If a study tests a hypothesis based on sound reasoning, using sound methodology with appropriate data analysis, those should be the absolute criteria for publishing," says Martin.

Despite not publishing articles on the basis of perceived impact and having a low overall rejection rate (~27%), the impact of Frontiers journals is significant and increasing. For example, Frontiers journals rank in the top 15 percent of impact factors for all journals, based on Journal Citation Reports published in 2016. The success of newer impact-neutral publishers is challenging the traditional belief that impact-based selectivity is the best way to publish impactful and quality research. Impact-neutral publishing may help researchers to address the replication crisis, by creating a positive atmosphere in which to publish replications.