Lab Manager | Run Your Lab Like a Business

News

A digital version of Tetris on a blue tinted screen
iStock, alengo

Tetris Reveals How People Respond to Unfair AI

Players who get fewer turns respond negatively to the other player even when the turns are assigned by an AI

by Cornell University
Register for free to listen to this article
Listen with Speechify
0:00
5:00

ITHACA, NY — A Cornell University-led experiment in which two people play a modified version of Tetris revealed that players who get fewer turns perceived the other player as less likable, regardless of whether a person or an algorithm allocated the turns.

Most studies on algorithmic fairness focus on the algorithm or the decision itself, but researchers sought to explore the relationships among the people affected by the decisions.

“We are starting to see a lot of situations in which AI makes decisions on how resources should be distributed among people,” said Malte Jung, associate professor of information science, whose group conducted the study. “We want to understand how that influences the way people perceive one another and behave towards each other. We see more and more evidence that machines mess with the way we interact with each other.”

In an earlier study, a robot chose which person to give a block to and studied the reactions of each individual to the machine’s allocation decisions.

“We noticed that every time the robot seemed to prefer one person, the other one got upset,” said Jung. “We wanted to study this further, because we thought that, as machines making decisions becomes more a part of the world—whether it be a robot or an algorithm—how does that make a person feel?”

Using open-source software, Houston Claure—the study’s first author and postdoctoral researcher at Yale University—developed a two-player version of Tetris, in which players manipulate falling geometric blocks in order to stack them without leaving gaps before the blocks pile to the top of the screen. Claure’s version, Co-Tetris, allows two people (one at a time) to work together to complete each round.

An “allocator”—either human or AI, which was conveyed to the players—determines which player takes each turn. Jung and Claure devised their experiment so that players would have either 90 percent of the turns (the “more” condition), 10 percent (“less”) or 50 percent (“equal”).

The researchers found, predictably, that those who received fewer turns were acutely aware that their partner got significantly more. But they were surprised to find that feelings about it were largely the same regardless of whether a human or an AI was doing the allocating.

The effect of these decisions is what the researchers have termed “machine allocation behavior”—similar to the established phenomenon of “resource allocation behavior,” the observable behavior people exhibit based on allocation decisions. Jung said machine allocation behavior is “the concept that there is this unique behavior that results from a machine making a decision about how something gets allocated.”

The researchers also found that fairness didn’t automatically lead to better game play and performance. In fact, equal allocation of turns led, on average, to a worse score than unequal allocation.

“If a strong player receives most of the blocks,” Claure said, “the team is going to do better. And if one person gets 90 percent, eventually they’ll get better at it than if two average players split the blocks.”

- This press release was provided by Cornell University